Social Psychology

Social Psychology Breakdown

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/47429343

Are Two Narcissists Better Than One? The Link Between Narcissism, Perceived Creativity, and Creative Performance

Article in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin · November 2010

DOI: 10.1177/0146167210385109 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS

69

READS

1,038

3 authors, including:

Jack A Goncalo

University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

33 PUBLICATIONS 1,332 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Jack A Goncalo on 16 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.

https://www.researchgate.net/publication/47429343_Are_Two_Narcissists_Better_Than_One_The_Link_Between_Narcissism_Perceived_Creativity_and_Creative_Performance?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/47429343_Are_Two_Narcissists_Better_Than_One_The_Link_Between_Narcissism_Perceived_Creativity_and_Creative_Performance?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jack_Goncalo?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jack_Goncalo?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Illinois_Urbana-Champaign?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jack_Goncalo?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jack_Goncalo?enrichId=rgreq-86526cc72b115d85c648e8ffc26297e5-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzQ3NDI5MzQzO0FTOjk3Mjk4NjQyMTEyNTE2QDE0MDAyMDkyMTI2MDc%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

http://psp.sagepub.com/

Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology

http://psp.sagepub.com/content/36/11/1484 The online version of this article can be found at:

DOI: 10.1177/0146167210385109

2010 36: 1484 originally published online 14 October 2010Pers Soc Psychol Bull Jack A. Goncalo, Francis J. Flynn and Sharon H. Kim

Performance Are Two Narcissists Better Than One? The Link Between Narcissism, Perceived Creativity, and Creative

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Society for Personality and Social Psychology

can be found at:Personality and Social Psychology BulletinAdditional services and information for

http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

http://psp.sagepub.com/content/36/11/1484.refs.htmlCitations:

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Article

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(11) 1484 –1495 © 2010 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0146167210385109 http://pspb.sagepub.com

Are Two Narcissists Better Than One? The Link Between Narcissism, Perceived Creativity, and Creative Performance

Jack A. Goncalo1, Francis J. Flynn2, and Sharon H. Kim1

Abstract

The current research examines the link between narcissism and creativity at the individual, relational, and group levels of analysis. It finds that narcissists are not necessarily more creative than others, but they think they are, and they are adept at persuading others to agree with them. In the first study, narcissism was positively associated with self-rated creativity, despite the fact that blind coders saw no difference between the creative products offered by those low and high on narcissism. In a second study, more narcissistic individuals asked to pitch creative ideas to a target person were judged by the targets as being more creative than were less narcissistic individuals, in part because narcissists were more enthusiastic. Finally, a study of group creativity finds evidence of a curvilinear effect: Having more narcissists is better for generating creative outcomes (but having too many provides diminishing returns).

Keywords

narcissism, creativity, self-enhancement, group processes

Received July 31, 2009; revision accepted June 7, 2010

God is really an artist, like me. . . . I am God, I am God, I am God.

Pablo Picasso

The above quote captures the stereotype of highly creative people as self-aggrandizing, self-indulgent, and self-absorbed. According to some scholars, such displays of narcissism may be an inevitable by-product of creative talent. Because creative people spend a great deal of time alone, are often absorbed in their work to the point of obsession, and refuse to conform to social conventions, they are likely to appear narcissistic to others (Barron & Harrington, 1981). In con- trast, some researchers propose that narcissism directly con- tributes to creativity because narcissists are motivated to generate novel ideas as a way to “stand out” and draw atten- tion to themselves (Raskin, 1980).

In the present research, we advance a different view. We argue that narcissists are not necessarily more creative than others, but they think they are, and they are adept at persuad- ing others to agree with them. Creativity is often judged by subjective evaluation rather than the satisfaction of objective criteria, both among practitioners (e.g., Sutton & Hargadon, 1996) and among scholars (e.g., Amabile, 1982). Given the ambiguity involved in judging creative work, narcissists may be particularly skillful not only at convincing themselves of

the high quality of their creative ideas but also at convey- ing their ideas with enough enthusiasm and confidence to impress their peers. Indeed, the traits that are typically asso- ciated with narcissism (e.g., self-confidence and self-esteem) may be well suited to support this social construction of their creative talents.

Although this paints a dim view of narcissists’ true cre- ativity, we do not mean to suggest that narcissism is irrele- vant to creative problem solving. Rather, we extend our analysis to the group level to suggest that narcissists are able to contribute to creative outcomes, but not on their own. Because narcissists crave attention for their contributions (John & Robbins, 1994), they may shift the entire group toward a more competitive norm that, in group settings, motivates idea expression (Dugosh & Paulus, 2005). In other words, narcissists may be highly effective at generating novel solutions to complex problems so long as there is at least one other narcissist in the group who can compete with him or her for attention and support of their opinions. To wit,

1Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA 2Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA

Corresponding Author: Jack A. Goncalo, Cornell University, School of Industrial and Labor Relations, 391 Ives Hall, Ithaca, NY 14853 Email: jag97@cornell.edu

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Goncalo et al. 1485

two narcissistic “heads” might be better than one because their tendency to engage in competitive dialogue bene- fits the group by prompting it to consider a wider range of potential solutions.

We tested these predictions in three studies that extend theory and research on narcissism and creativity in a number of important ways. First, we provide evidence of a link between narcissism and creativity that is akin to a social construction—narcissists are skillful in persuading others (as well as themselves) that they have creative ideas even when they do not. Second, we build on the extant narcissism litera- ture by advancing a counterintuitive hypothesis—that nar- cissistic group members can inspire higher levels of creative performance from his or her colleagues, although the source of the group’s creative output may not necessarily be the result of the narcissist’s own creative contributions. Third, and more generally, we contribute to a broad scholarly interest in the determinants of creativity, demonstrating that narcissists are indeed linked to highly creative outcomes, but not owing to the fact that they are highly creative people.

Narcissism and Individual Creativity Narcissism refers to a set of egocentric traits including self- admiration, self-centeredness, and self-regard (Sedikides, Rudich, Gregg, Kumashiro, & Rusbult, 2004). Individuals scoring high in narcissism have a strong sense of entitlement and a constant need for attention and admiration (Bogart, Benotsch, & Pavlovic, 2004). They frequently use singular personal pronouns (e.g., I, me) in speech (Raskin & Shaw, 1988) and fail to listen attentively to others (Kernis & Sun, 1994). Narcissists report a lesser need for intimacy (Carroll, 1987) and have little empathy for their peers, even those in distress (Watson, Grisham, Trotter, & Biderman, 1984). Perhaps more surprisingly, narcissists tend to emerge as leaders (Brunell et al., 2008), even at the highest levels of organizations (Chatterjee & Hambrick, 2007).

Descriptions of highly creative people indicate that they are often highly narcissistic—driven only by their own desires and insensitive to the opinions of others (e.g., MacDonald & Wilson, 2005). However, anecdotal accounts linking narcis- sism to creativity are hard to interpret because it is difficult to disentangle narcissists’ objective creative performance from their own and others’ perceptions of their creativity. On one hand, narcissism might be a trait, like openness to expe- rience (McCrae, 1987), that predicts performance on creative tasks. For example, the tendency of narcissists to use I pro- nouns in speech might be indicative of a differentiation mind- set that has been shown to stimulate divergent thinking (Wiekens & Stapel, 2008).

On the other hand, there is evidence to suggest that narcis- sists may not be more adept at coming up with creative ideas, just more likely to overestimate their creativity relative to others. Narcissists are self-aggrandizers; that is, they tend to

give themselves too much credit for their past accomplish- ments and are overly optimistic about their future success (John & Robbins, 1994). In a sample of undergraduate stu- dents, narcissism correlated strongly with the grades that par- ticipants expected to receive in their courses (i.e., narcissists predicted they would receive higher grades), although there was no correlation between narcissism and undergraduates’ actual course grades (Farwell & Wohlwend-Lloyd, 2008).

Building on this research, we expect to find significant self-enhancement from narcissists (relative to others) when asked to judge their own performance on a creative task. Most people, narcissistic or not, find it difficult to generate creative solutions because they are often constrained by their prior experiences (Duncker, 1945; Ward, 1994). Narcissists may be subject to similar constraints, but their tendency toward self-enhancement will make them less likely than others to recognize that their ideas are not especially novel. In other words, we do not expect to find a positive relation between narcissism and individual creativity. Instead, we expect to find significant self-enhancement from narcissists when asked to judge their creative talents.

Narcissism and Perceptions of Creative Talent Narcissists may not be creative, but their high levels of self- confidence may nevertheless influence the way others evalu- ate their ideas. Although researchers have numerous tools at their disposal for measuring creativity, there are many con- texts in which creativity is judged by observers who lack rig- orous criteria (Amabile, 1982; Taylor & Barron, 1963) and are subject to attributional biases (Kasof, 1995). For exam- ple, in a qualitative study of Hollywood “pitches,” Elsbach and Kramer (2003) found that judgments of creativity were influenced by perceptions of the “pitcher” and the extent to which they matched the prototypical traits of a highly cre- ative person, such as “charismatic” and “witty.” As one studio executive explained, “someone who is enthusiastic and passionate can make a regular story sound spectacular” (Elsbach & Kramer, 2003, p. 290).

This quote is revealing for two reasons. First, it highlights the fact that perceptions of creative ability may be separate from whether a product is objectively creative. Second, it suggests that certain behaviors of the person who pitches creative ideas, especially their energy, enthusiasm, and con- viction, can prompt evaluators to judge their ideas to be more creative than they actually are. This second point dovetails with research on social influence in which behav- iors that signal confidence, such as taking the head seat before a group discussion, can make one’s ideas seem more plausible and convincing (Nemeth & Wachtler, 1974). More recent research also suggests that dominant individuals are more likely to attain social status in groups because oth- ers inaccurately perceive them as more competent (Anderson & Kilduff, 2009).

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

1486 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(11)

We expect that narcissists may be at a significant advan- tage in these evaluations because they will be both highly confident that they are more creative than others and more inclined to publicly share these flattering self-views with people who are in a position to evaluate their ideas. In the absence of any objective information about an idea’s cre- ative quality or criteria on which to base such an evaluation, narcissists’ self-aggrandizing behaviors may be persuasive, particularly because they match evaluators’ prototypes of how highly creative people tend to behave (Elsbach & Kramer, 2003). This social influence process, more than the objective creativity of the idea itself, could help explain why narcissists have been described as “visionaries” by people who have observed them in innovative contexts (Deutschman, 2005, p. 44).

Narcissism and Group Creativity We claim that narcissism may not stimulate individual cre- ativity, but what about the link between narcissism and cre- ative performance in groups? Here we extend our analysis to the group level by addressing the following question: When it comes to creativity, are two (or more) narcissists better than one? Group creativity depends heavily on the open expression of ideas because people may extend, combine, and improve on the contributions made by others (Nijstad, Stroebe, & Lodewijkx, 2002). Unfortunately, many good ideas remain unexpressed, leading groups to underperform compared to individuals who work alone (Diehl & Stroebe, 1987). Competition can serve as an effective stimulant of creative ideas because the need for superiority motivates people to express ideas they might otherwise withhold from the group discussion (Dugosh & Paulus, 2005; Munkes & Diehl, 2003).

Consistent with this perspective, research on social motives has shown that groups of people with a pro-self orientation (i.e., the goal is to maximize one’s own outcomes relative to others) are more creative than groups of people with a pro- social orientation (i.e., the goal is to cooperate to maximize outcomes for both oneself and others; Beersma & De Dreu, 2005). In a similar vein, groups of people primed to be indi- vidualistic generated more novel ideas than groups of people primed to be collectivistic (Goncalo & Staw, 2006). Taken together, these streams of research suggest that the creative potential of groups may be realized when the drive to be supe- rior compels each group member to attempt to propose the most novel ideas (Beersma & De Dreu, 2005; Dugosh & Paulus, 2005; Munkes & Diehl, 2003). Given that narcissists crave attention and recognition for their valued attributes and con- tributions (e.g., John & Robbins, 1994), competition between narcissistic group members may lead the group to uncover new sources of information and new perspectives that can then be recombined to generate novel ideas (De Dreu, Nijstad, & van Knippenberg, 2008).

In particular, narcissists may actually contribute to a more efficient exchange of ideas by reducing production blocking (Diehl & Stroebe, 1987). Production blocking is caused by group members listening to other group members’ ideas and waiting until the other person has finished before expressing their idea (Nijstad et al., 2002). Highly narcis- sistic individuals may be less patient with such turn taking (and tend to “break into” the other person’s turn) or not lis- ten as attentively to the other person’s ideas (and thereby be less likely to forget their own ideas). This self-focus could reduce production blocking and thereby increase the group’s creative output.1 Indeed, there is recent evidence that people in competitive groups are more likely to interrupt their teammates to express their own ideas and that doing so actu- ally increases the total number of ideas expressed (Goncalo & Kim, 2010).

Yet, the relation between narcissism and creativity in groups may be more complex than a direct linear associa- tion. As more narcissists join the mix, competition can escalate to the point of obstructing the group’s ability to reach closure, synthesize new ideas, and complete tasks on time (Jehn & Mannix, 2001). Groups with lower levels of competition may be more efficient and more capable of coordinating their efforts, which would be an advantage when the group moves beyond the idea-generation stage to actually select an idea and bring it to fruition (Rietzschel, Nijstad, & Stroebe, 2006). Given these trade-offs, we pre- dict a curvilinear, inverted U-shaped relation: The more narcissists there are in the group, the more creative the group’s performance will be up to an inflection point when addi- tional narcissists begin to have a negative effect on group creativity.

Summary of Predictions and Overview of Studies We report the results of three studies in which we investi- gated the link between narcissism and creativity at the indi- vidual, relational, and group levels of analysis. In Study 1, we draw on two classic creativity tests to examine our pre- diction that narcissists are not necessarily more creative than non-narcissists but that they nonetheless judge their own efforts as being more creative. In Study 2, we look at how observers evaluate the creativity of others’ ideas. Although narcissists do not necessarily generate more cre- ative ideas, they may be able to convince others that these ideas are more creative because their high levels of confidence, enthusiasm, and charisma correspond to commonly held pro- totypes of the creative personality (Katz & Giacommelli, 1982; Runco & Bahleda, 1986). Finally, in Study 3, we turn to the generation of creative ideas in a group context. We expect that groups with more narcissists will be more cre- ative, but as narcissists represent a greater proportion of group membership, their positive influence on group cre- ativity will diminish.

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Goncalo et al. 1487

ideas on a lined sheet. On the second task, we asked partici- pants to “imagine going to another galaxy in the universe and visiting a planet very different from Earth” (Ward, 1994). Participants were then given 7 min to draw a picture of an animal that is “local to this other planet.”

After completing each task, participants were instructed to fill out a brief questionnaire in which they were asked to evaluate the creativity of their own work. They assessed their performance on the Alternate Uses Test by indicating the extent to which they agreed with each of the following four statements: (a) “The alternative uses for a brick I came up with were highly creative,” (b) “I probably came up with at least one use for a brick that no one else in this class came up with,” (c) “My performance on the uses for a brick test prob- ably shows that I am more creative than most people,” and (d) “The alternative uses for a brick I came up with are prob- ably very conventional” (reverse scored). Responses to the scale were reliable (α = .80) and so they were averaged together (M = 3.14, SD = .89). Participants also assessed their performance on the structured imagination task by responding to the same set of statements (e.g., “The space creature I drew was highly creative”). Each item was rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely). Responses to the scale were moderately reliable (α = .78). Therefore, we averaged participants’ responses to these questions to create an overall measure of perceived creativity (M = 2.98, SD = .92).

Creativity coding. The Alternate Uses Test was scored by first counting the sheer number of uses listed (fluency) and then coding the ideas for the extent to which the solutions were qualitatively different from each other (flexibility; Guilford, 1956). Fluency was assessed by a direct count of the number of alternatives listed. Flexibility was calculated based on the ratings of two coders who were blind to the hypotheses of the study. The coders independently sorted the entire sample of ideas (n = 4,571) into categories based on how similar they were to each other; for instance, all the ideas suggesting that the brick be used to build something were placed in one category, and so on. We then counted the number of categories of ideas generated by each participant. Coders reached significant agreement on the number of cat- egories covered by each individual (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] = .86, p < .01); therefore their category counts were averaged together (M = 10.71, SD = 4.19). Par- ticipants who generated ideas that crossed many categories had higher flexibility than participants whose ideas crossed fewer categories. People who are able to think divergently generate more ideas that cross more categories.

Following directly from previous research (e.g., Ward, 1994), structured imagination was coded from the atypical- ity of the space creatures’ sensory organs. Three trained coders who were blind to the study hypotheses assessed the drawings and accompanying descriptions for evidence of “atypical” sensory organs. Following Ward’s (1994) original

Study 1 Method

Participants. Participants were 244 undergraduates from a large university on the east coast of the United States who participated in exchange for partial course credit. Men com- prised 52% of the sample.2

Narcissism measure. To assess each participant’s level of narcissism, we used the abridged Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-16) created by Ames, Rose, and Anderson (2006). According to the authors, the NPI-16 is a short mea- sure of subclinical narcissism that has shown meaningful face, internal, discriminant, and predictive validity. Using items that are drawn from the longer Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-40) developed by Raskin and Terry (1988), the NPI-16 instructs respondents to read 16 pairs of state- ments and choose the one that comes closest to describing their true feelings or beliefs. A sample pair of descriptive statements would read: “I really like to be the center of atten- tion” and “It makes me uncomfortable to be the center of attention.” Each of the 16 pairs has one statement that is con- sistent with narcissism and one that is not. The scale is scored by counting the number of responses consistent with narcis- sism (M = 6.74, SD = 3.11). The scale was moderately reli- able (α = .72).

Creativity tasks. We measured creativity using two tasks: the Alternate Uses Test (Guilford, 1956) and Ward’s (1994) measure of structured imagination. First, the Alternate Uses Test assesses the ability to think divergently, defined as the generation of solutions that move outward from a problem in many different directions (Guilford, 1956). People who think divergently are able to generate a large number of ideas (fluency) that are different from one another (flexibility; Guilford, 1956). Wallace and Baumeister (2002) employed the Alternate Uses Test to investigate a link between narcis- sism and task effort, and therefore focused only on the sheer number of ideas generated. We extend this research by inves- tigating the question of whether narcissists generated alter- nate uses that were in fact more divergent. Second, Ward’s (1994) measure of structured imagination gauges the extent to which people can overcome the constraints of past experi- ence to generate a product that represents a novel departure from existing knowledge. Most people find it difficult to overcome these constraints. For instance, people who are instructed to imagine space creatures that are different “beyond their wildest” imaginations often come up with aliens that have human characteristics such as bilateral symmetry (Ward, 1994).

Two weeks after participants completed the NPI-16, they were given the creativity tasks (we created a delay between these measures to minimize demand effects). On the first task, we asked participants to generate as many alternative uses for a brick as possible in 10 min (Guilford, 1956). No further instructions were given. Participants recorded their

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

1488 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(11)

coding scheme, space creatures were considered atypical if they (a) lacked a major sensory organ (i.e., eyes, ears, nose), (b) had atypical numbers of a sensory organ (e.g., three eyes), (c) demonstrated an unusual configuration of the senses (e.g., eyes located below the nose), (d) had an exag- gerated or unusual ability (e.g., eyes that had laser beams), or (e) served an atypical function (e.g., ears for protection). The total number of atypical features was tallied for each partici- pant. The ratings of the two coders reached significant agree- ment (ICC = .77, p < .01) and so their ratings were averaged together to create an overall measure of creative performance (M = 3.10, SD = 1.58).

Results and Discussion A bivariate correlation showed that narcissism was not sig- nificantly associated with fluency (r = .08, ns) or flexibility (r = .06, ns). To investigate whether more narcissistic indi- viduals would view their own ideas as more creative, regard- less of their objective performance, we conducted linear regressions in which we controlled for fluency and flexibility (these variables were significantly correlated, r = .85, p < .05). As predicted, narcissism was positively associated with self-ratings of creativity when controlling for fluency (β = .22, p < .01) and flexibility (β = .24 p < .05), even though, as one would expect, both fluency (β = .42, p < .01) and flexi- bility (β = .43, p < .01) were themselves significantly associ- ated with self-rated creativity.

Despite these results it is possible that narcissists were not necessarily focused on all of their ideas and whether they were different from each other but on whether they generated a few ideas—even one idea they perceived to be extremely original. To address this possibility we conducted an addi- tional analysis in which we counted the number of ideas gen- erated that were so unusual the coders were not able to categorize them. An example of one such idea is, “Use a brick to cast a shadow.” The results, however, showed that there was also no significant correlation between narcissism and the number of original ideas generated (r = –.17, ns).

We observed the same pattern of results on the structured imagination task. Narcissism was not significantly associated with the number of atypical features in the space alien draw- ings (r = –.05, ns). However, as predicted, there was a signifi- cant association between narcissism and participants’ ratings of the creativity of their own drawings (r = .27, p < .01), even when controlling for the number of atypical features, which was also a significant predictor of self-ratings of creativity (r = .20, p < .01).

Together, these results indicate that narcissists (or those who score relatively higher on a standardized measure of subclinical narcissism) saw their own performance as being more creative, unique, and novel, although an assessment made by independent judges revealed no discernible differ- ence on these dimensions.

Study 2

In Study 2 we investigated whether more narcissistic people are perceived to be more creative than less narcissistic peo- ple because their confidence and enthusiasm matches the prototypes people have about highly creative individuals. We predict that highly narcissistic individuals will suggest ideas that are not objectively more creative but are perceived by evaluators as more creative than those suggested by indi- viduals who are less narcissistic.

Method Participants. Participants were 76 students from a large

university on the west coast of the United States who com- pleted the study in exchange for course credit. Men comprised 64% of the sample.

Procedure. Participants were randomly assigned to an experimental session and arrived in pairs. At the beginning of the study, the experimenter randomly selected one person to assume the role of the pitcher and the other to assume the role of the evaluator. Each participant’s role was explained directly to him or her after being seated alone in a separate room. The participant who was randomly selected to assume the pitcher role was told:

We are interested in how people behave during pitch meetings when one person tries to sell their ideas to another person. In this study we will focus on new movie ideas and you have been assigned to play the role of the “pitcher.” This role involves (1) coming up with a new movie idea, (2) developing and rehearsing a pitch that you will use to sell your idea to an evalua- tor and (3) actually pitching your idea.

Pitchers were also told that the ideas they generated would be scored by their evaluators and that the pitcher who received the highest score (across all the experimental sessions) would receive an additional cash prize of $50. In fact, at the end of the experimental session, one participant was randomly awarded the $50 prize, although debriefing conversations indicated that all the participants believed the cash prize was real and felt motivated to obtain it.

After receiving their instructions, pitchers were given 10 min to come up with a new movie idea and to rehearse their pitch. Each was provided with five sheets of scratch paper and a pen and informed that the actual pitch would last no longer than 10 min.

The experimenter then entered the room in which the evaluator was seated and informed him or her:

We are interested in how people behave during pitch meetings when one person tries to sell their ideas to another person. In this study we will focus on new

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Goncalo et al. 1489

movie ideas and you have been assigned to play the role of the “evaluator.” This role involves (1) listening to someone pitch a new movie idea, and (2) deciding whether you, as an evaluator, think the idea is good enough to actually produce.

Each participant was then given 10 min to think about the criteria they would use to evaluate the new movie idea and to write the criteria on the scratch paper provided.

After 10 min elapsed, the pitcher was escorted into the next room and seated across the table from the evaluator. Both were then told:

As you already know, we are interested in how people behave during pitch meetings when one person tries to sell their ideas to another person. In this study we are interested in new movie ideas and one of you has been assigned to play the role of the pitcher and the other the role of evaluator. You will have 10 minutes to make your pitch. Evaluator, please listen to the pitch silently. You will be asked to make your judgment about the idea after the pitch is complete.

The experimenter returned after 10 min elapsed and escorted the pitcher back to the rehearsal room. In the meantime, the evaluators were instructed to sit quietly and informed that the experimenter would return with a survey. Once the pitcher had been seated in a separate room, the evaluators were then given 10 min to complete a brief questionnaire in which they were asked to rate the creativity of the movie idea and their impressions of the individual who pitched it.

Narcissism measure. Once again, we relied on the NPI-16 to measure participants’ levels of narcissism (Ames et al., 2006). Specifically, we asked the pitchers to complete this measure and scored their responses in the same manner as described in Study 1 (M = 5.48, SD = 2.84). The reliability of the scale was modest (α = .65) but similar to the scale reli- ability reported in previous research (Ames et al., 2006).

Creativity ratings. The evaluators were asked to rate the creativity of the movie idea that was pitched to them by responding to the following four items: (a) “The movie idea is creative,” (b) “This movie idea is more creative than the movies that have been at the theaters lately,” (c) “Other peo- ple will think that this movie idea is creative,” and (d) “It is unlikely that anyone has come up with a movie idea like this before.” Each of these four items was rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scale was moderately reliable (α = .79) and so the items were averaged to generate an overall measure of perceived creativity (M = 3.80, SD = 1.18).

Creative personality type. The evaluators were also asked to rate their impressions of the pitcher’s “energy” by respond- ing to the following four items: (a) “The pitcher was charis- matic,” (b) “The pitcher was witty,” (c) “The pitcher was

extreme,” and (d) “The pitcher was enthusiastic.” Each of these four individual traits was rated on a 7-point scale rang- ing from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scale was moderately reliable (α = .75) and so the items were averaged to create an overall measure of perceived cre- ative personality (M = 3.74, SD = .96). We chose to focus on these four personal characteristics because they have been shown in previous research to correspond to prototypes that people hold about highly creative personalities. In particular, these characteristics have been shown to predict attributions of creativity in the context of Hollywood pitch meetings (see Elsbach & Kramer, 2003, for a complete description).

Control variables. Despite the results of Study 1, we sought to rule out the possibility that the narcissists in our second study may have been able to generate movie ideas that are objectively more creative. To test this alternative explana- tion, we evaluated the creativity of the movie ideas based on the pitchers’ written descriptions. Following the definition of a creative idea as one that is both novel and feasible (Amabile, 1982), two blind coders independently rated each pitch using two 5-point scales ranging from 1 (not at all fea- sible) to 5 (extremely feasible) and from 1 (not at all novel) to 5 (extremely novel). The coders reached significant agreement on both their ratings of novelty (ICC = .81) and feasibility (ICC = .92) so their scores were averaged together. In addi- tion, we also controlled for the sex composition of the dyad to rule out the possibility that evaluations were driven by sex dif- ferences (Mannix & Neale, 2005). In our analysis, mixed-sex dyads were coded 1 and same-sex dyads were coded 0.

Results Perceptions of creativity. Consistent with our prediction,

narcissism was significantly correlated with the evaluators’ rating of creativity (β = .30, p < .05), controlling for the sex composition of the dyad (β = .35, p < .05). We also expected that narcissists would be perceived by evaluators as having personal characteristics that match the prototype of a highly creative personality. In line with this prediction, narcissism was significantly correlated with the prototype of a creative personality (β = .32, p < .05), controlling for the sex compo- sition of the dyad (β = .34, p < .05).

To replicate the results of Study 1, we also investigated the possibility that narcissists may have generated more cre- ative ideas. Given that creativity is defined as an idea that is both novel and feasible, we averaged the novelty and feasi- bility ratings to create a composite measure. Again, narcis- sism was not correlated with the creativity of the ideas pitched (r = –.00, ns). Novelty and feasibility are often negatively correlated (e.g., Rietzschel, Nijstad, & Stroebe, 2010), and the same pattern emerged in our data as well (r = –.42, ns); therefore, we also analyzed novelty and feasibility sepa- rately. Again, the results showed no correlation between nar- cissism and the novelty of the ideas pitched (r = .22, ns),

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

1490 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(11)

nor between narcissism and the feasibility of the ideas pitched (r = –.24, ns). Consistent with the results of Study 1, there was no indication that narcissism contributed to actual creative performance.

Mediation analysis. The results indicate that narcissists pitched ideas that were perceived to be more creative than the ideas pitched by non-narcissists. We wanted to see if this effect was mediated by evaluators’ impressions of narcis- sists’ traits. We followed the procedures recommended by Baron and Kenny (1986) to test for mediation. The indepen- dent variable was narcissism (1 = high, 0 = low), the media- tor was the evaluator’s impression of the pitcher’s energy (e.g., charismatic, enthusiastic), and the dependent variable was the evaluator’s rating of the creativity of the movie idea.

First, narcissism (independent variable) was positively related to impressions of the pitcher’s energy (mediator), β = .32, p < .05. Second, narcissism (independent variable) was positively related to the evaluator’s rating of the movie idea’s creativity (dependent variable; β = .30, p < .05). Third, impressions of the pitcher’s energy (mediator) was signifi- cantly related to the rated creativity of the movie ideas (dependent variable; β = .50, p < .01). Finally, when both nar- cissism (independent variable) and impressions of the pitch- er’s energy (mediator) were entered into the equation simultaneously, narcissism was not significant (β = .16, ns), and impressions of the pitcher’s energy remained significant (β = .44, p < .01). Given the small sample size, standard pro- cedures advocate the use of a bootstrap analysis to calculate bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals to evalu- ate mediation effects (Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). The analysis yielded a confidence interval around the mediation effect that did not include zero, reveal- ing that the mediation effect was significant, β = –.35, 95% CI [–.13, –.01], 1,000 bootstrap resamples. Thus, we can con- clude that evaluators’ impressions of the pitcher’s energy and enthusiasm mediated the effect of narcissism on evaluations of creativity. Figure 1 summarizes the mediation process.

Discussion In sum, these findings suggest that narcissists may be effec- tive at convincing others that their ideas are creative, in part because they convey traits that are closely associated with a creative personality prototype. In specific terms, because narcissists come across as more charismatic, enthusiastic, and energetic, they can convince their audience that the ideas they advocate are more novel than those advocated by non- narcissists who have ideas that are equally creative but con- veyed with less personal force. We also addressed a potential alternative explanation for the findings in Study 1. It is pos- sible that we did not observe performance differences on the creativity tasks because there was no potential evaluation or reward associated with doing well in that study (Wallace & Baumeister, 2002). However, the results of Study 2 show

that narcissism is not associated with creativity, even when the task has an explicit evaluative component built into it. Therefore, we can have greater confidence that narcissists’ evaluations of their own creativity are in fact the result of self-enhancement.

Study 3 In Study 3, we test the intriguing possibility that having more narcissists in a group can help stimulate collaborative cre- ativity, until a point at which it becomes a detriment.

Method Sample and procedure. Participants were 292 undergraduate

students from an introductory course in organizational psychol- ogy who completed the study for partial course credit. Men comprised 53% of the sample. Each person was randomly assigned to groups of four, resulting in a total of 73 teams.

Each team was asked to analyze a real organization making use of the concepts and methods highlighted in the course. Specifically, they were told to “adopt the clinical pose of a management consultant, endeavoring to understand the organization, to identify its strengths and weaknesses and ultimately to propose actions that solve problems and improve performance.” The portion of the paper that is most relevant to this study is the section in which teams proposed a solution to the problem they identified. In this section, groups were instructed to generate novel plans that the orga- nization could implement to improve their problems and build on their strengths. The solutions were not intended to be wild or unrealistic. In fact, they were explicitly instructed to come up with feasible action items—things the organiza- tion could do given its constraints. Students took these proj- ects seriously because they accounted for 40% of their overall course grade.

β = .50 t = 3.40 β = .32 t = 2.06 p < .05

β = .44 t = 2.86

β = .30 t = 2.20

β = .16 t = 1.09

p < .05

p = .29

Without impressions

With impressions

Without narcissism

With narcissism

Narcissism

Perceived Enthusiasm

Evaluation of Creativity

p < .01

p < .01

Figure 1. Main and mediating effects of narcissism, impressions of the pitcher, and evaluations of creativity Dotted arrow indicates that a relationship fell below significance in the full model (e.g., there is full mediation).

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Goncalo et al. 1491

At the beginning of the semester, students were told that their research participation was voluntary and anony- mous, and that the information they provided would not be made available to their instructor. Surveys designed to assess the creative process were handed out at the midpoint of the group project, halfway between the assignment to groups and the final deadline. We chose the midpoint because pre- vious research has shown that the midpoint is when high- performing groups experience a concentrated burst of activity, at which time they debate competing task-related perspec- tives (Gersick, 1988). Therefore, it is at this stage of a group’s development when the creative process might be most relevant.

Independent variable. Our primary independent variable was the average narcissism score of each group. At the beginning of the semester, participants completed the NPI-16 before being assigned to a group project team (M = 6.79, SD = 1.67). The reliability of the scale was α = .72.

Dependent variable. Assessments of individual creativity typically focus either on the process of being creative or on an objective product that can be rated by outside observers (Amabile, 1982). Scholars who focus on the former typically assess cognitive processes that are believed to be associated with creative problem solving (Simonton, 2003). For instance, the creative process at the individual level requires some capacity to generate novel alternatives or remote associa- tions, and these abilities are assessed using creativity tests (Guilford, 1967; Mednick, 1962). Scholars who take the lat- ter approach typically use outside raters to judge the creativ- ity of products (e.g., Amabile, 1982; MacKinnon, 1962).

This distinction between process and product is relevant to group creativity as well. A creative group process requires sys- tematic information processing whereby a wide range of ideas are both expressed and thoughtfully deliberated (De Dreu et al., 2008), whereas a creative group product is one that can be rated by outside observers on the extent to which it represents a novel and appropriate solution (Amabile, 1982). With this dis- tinction in mind, we decided to measure group creativity on both dimensions following from our prediction that the pres- ence of narcissists should motivate the group to consider more task-related alternatives (systematic thinking) and deliver a solution that is a departure from the status quo (creative prod- uct). These measures are described in detail next.

Systematic thinking. Each group member responded to four survey statements: (a) “My group tries to consider all possi- ble alternatives before making decisions,” (b) “My group is extremely thorough when making decisions,” (c) “My group debates many ideas before making decisions,” and (d) “My group thinks deeply before making decisions” drawn from previous research (see De Dreu et al., 2008, for a review). Group members responded on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). This measure was computed by aggregating data gathered at the individual level (Kozlowski & Klein, 2000). Therefore, we calculated

two indicators of within-group agreement to justify aggrega- tion: the rWG within-group agreement measure (James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984) and the ICC (1) score (James, 1982). Following previous research (Kozlowski & Klein, 2000), we adopted a cutoff of .70 or higher for the rWG and a cutoff of .20 for the ICC (1). The rWG was .85 and ICC (1) was .77 (F = 9.97, p < .01), indicating significant within- group agreement on the group’s creative process, thus jus- tifying the aggregation of individual scores to the group level (M = 2.53, SD = 1.14).

Group product. The creativity of each group’s project was rated by two independent coders who were blind to our hypotheses. Specifically, the coders rated the extent to which each solution was either radical or incremental because, assuming the solutions are practical, creative solutions are characteristically novel in the sense that they depart from existing approaches (Simonton, 2003). Therefore, each team’s term paper was coded on the following 5-point scale: 1 = extremely incremental (no change or almost no change rec- ommended), 2 = incremental (something that can already be done within the current system), 3 = neither radical nor incremental, 4 = radical (a proposal that required a major change), and 5 = extremely radical (a change that would completely overhaul the organization’s current approach). The coders reached significant agreement (ICC = .75, p < .01) and so their scores were averaged to create a single measure of group creativity (M = 2.60, SD = 1.66).

Control variable. It is possible that groups with more nar- cissistic members were less cooperative. As a consequence, these teams may have chosen to meet less frequently to dis- cuss the project during the semester and adopted a more independent work style that influenced group creativity in a positive way (Nemeth & Goncalo, 2005). To rule out these effects, we asked each group member to estimate the total number of times they met to discuss the project, averaged their estimates (M = 4.60, SD = 1.41), and controlled for it in our analyses.

Results and Discussion We analyzed the data using two hierarchical linear regres- sions in which the control variable was entered on the first step and the independent variables were entered on the sec- ond step. Because we predicted a curvilinear effect of narcis- sism on group creativity, we created a quadratic term by squaring the number of narcissists in each group and included this quadratic term in each regression analysis. Finally, we also controlled for the standard deviation of narcissism in each group, in addition to the mean, to control for within- group variation. This approach follows that used in several previous research studies (e.g., Barrick, Stewart, Neubert, & Mount, 1998).

In Model 1, the dependent variable is the group’s creative process. The meeting frequency variable was not significant

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

1492 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(11)

(β = .02, ns), and neither was the standard deviation of nar- cissism within each group (β = –.22, ns). The results showed a positive but only marginally significant linear effect of nar- cissism on group creativity (β = 2.04, p < .10) and a signifi- cant curvilinear effect of narcissism on group creativity (β = –2.27, p < .01). The coefficient of the quadratic term was negative, indicating that the relation between narcissism and creativity had an inverted U-shape. The inflection point was calculated by taking the partial derivative as recommended by Aiken and West (1991). The inflection point equaled 6.48. In other words, group creativity increased as the mean level of narcissism in the group increased up to a point approaching a narcissism score of 7, at which point group creativity began to diminish (see Figure 2).

In Model 2, the dependent variable is the groups’ cre- ative product. The meeting frequency variable was not sig- nificant (β = .09, ns), and neither was the standard deviation of narcissism in each group (β = –.07, ns). The linear effect of the narcissism measure on group product creativity was positive and significant (β = 1.63, p = .01) and the curvilin- ear effect of narcissism on group product creativity was negative and significant (β = –1.92, p < .01). The coefficient of the quadratic term indicates that the relation between nar- cissism and creativity had an inverted U-shape. Again, we calculated the inflection point, which equaled 6.13. That is, the creativity of the group product increased as the mean level of narcissism in each group increased up to a narcis- sism score of approximately 6, at which point group creativ- ity began to diminish (see Figure 3). Taken together, these results suggest that the creativity of both the group process and product were facilitated by the presence of more narcissis- tic individuals up to a point at which increasing narcissism became detrimental.

General Discussion We argued that narcissists are not necessarily more creative than other people but simply think they are. Nevertheless, the enthusiasm with which they “sell” their ideas may elicit more favorable evaluations of creativity relative to their less narcissistic peers (Kasof, 1995). Consistent with this predic- tion, we found in Study 1 that narcissists evaluated their own creative talents more positively than did non-narcissists, but their alleged creativity was not supported by objective mea- sures of their performance. The results of Study 2, however, demonstrated that evaluators believed the ideas pitched by narcissists were more creative, and these perceptions were fully mediated by their impressions of narcissists as enthusi- astic and charismatic. Therefore, in contexts where there are no objective standards for judging creativity, narcissists may be adept at getting people to share their inflated self-views.

In Study 3 we extended our investigation to the group level of analysis where the creative process becomes interac- tive and requires the motivation to fully explore and consider

alternative points of view (De Dreu et al., 2008). On two measures of group creativity, one that focused on systematic thinking and the other on the product itself, groups with approximately two narcissistic members (out of four) out- performed groups with too many or too few. In short, narcis- sists can contribute to creativity in groups even if they may not perform creatively while working alone. The notion that more narcissists are better for group creativity is counterin- tuitive, certainly more counterintuitive than the notion that groups with a higher percentage of members who are open to new experience and tolerant of ambiguity tend to be more creative (Baer, Oldham, Jacobsohn, & Hollingshead, 2008). But the same needs for recognition and power that cast a

1.00

1.50

2.00

2.50

3.00

3.50

4.00

4.50

5.00

0.00 3.00 6.00 9.00 12.00

S ys

te m

at ic

T h

in ki

n g

Number of Narcissists

Figure 2. The curvilinear effects of narcissistic group composition on systematic thinking in groups Systematic thinking increases as the number of narcissists increase to an inflection point of 6.48.

1.00

1.50

2.00

2.50

3.00

3.50

4.00

4.50

5.00

0.00 3.00 6.00 9.00 12.00

G ro

u p

C re

at iv

it y

Average Group Narcissism Score

Figure 3. The curvilinear effects of narcissistic group composition on group creativity Group creativity increases as the average group narcissism score increase to an inflection point of 6.13.

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Goncalo et al. 1493

dark shadow on narcissists may position them as catalysts for creative colloquy.

Theoretical Contributions and Future Directions Our results build on existing research in several important ways. First, we draw attention to the link between narcissism and attributions of creativity (Kasof, 1995). Garnering rec- ognition for one’s creative achievements requires some skill in the art of persuasion, particularly in less paradigmatic fields in which there are fewer objective standards to deter- mine whether one idea is more creative than another (Kuhn, 1962). Future research might investigate other traits that make some people especially skilled at this endeavor. For instance, high self-monitors may be able to convince people their ideas are creative because they are able to detect subtle cues about how creativity is evaluated in different contexts (Snyder, 1974).

Our findings that narcissists are viewed as more creative than an objective evaluation of their work would warrant may have potentially disturbing implications for the way the process of evaluating ideas plays out over time. In most fields there is a selection process whereby some ideas are determined to be highly creative and therefore worthy of being disseminated and adopted while other ideas are deemed not worthy of such attention (Csikszentmihalyi, 1988). Ideally, this process would be relatively objective, and higher quality ideas should have a higher probability of selection than oth- ers (Simonton, 2003). Our results, however, suggest that this process can be contaminated when evaluations of cre- ativity are overly influenced by the behavioral style with which they are communicated, particularly in fields that lack objective performance criteria (Barron, 1965). In such fields, creative output may gradually decline as true cre- ative talent is continuously traded for charisma and enthusiasm.

Creativity is a complex and multifaceted construct, and there are many ways to measure it; therefore, it would be unrealistic to claim that our conclusions regarding the link between narcissism and creativity are definitive. For instance, Raskin (1980) reported a small but significant correlation between narcissism and the Barron Symbolic Equivalence Test (1967), though it should be noted that this relation did not remain significant when controlling for self-reported cre- ativity. Nevertheless, future research might examine the link between narcissism and creative performance using other tasks and perhaps longitudinal methods in which narcissism is linked to creative achievement over time (e.g., Helson, Roberts, & Agronick, 1995).

Future research might also differentiate between the influence of narcissism on creativity and the influence of other forms of positive self-evaluation such as self-esteem, self-acceptance, self-confidence, or self-efficacy (Tierney & Farmer, 2002). Such traits might be mistaken for narcissism

in social interactions, but whereas these other traits have established links to individual creativity, narcissism does not. This may be why the stereotype of highly creative people as narcissistic is so enduring: Legitimately creative people may often be viewed as narcissistic when this is not the case, and narcissists often pass as highly creative when they may in fact lack creative talent.

Finally, the results of Study 3 demonstrated a curvilinear effect of narcissistic group composition with an inflection point at two narcissistic group members. Future research might investigate groups of varying size to determine whether having two narcissists in a group of any size is sufficient to produce these benefits or whether it is critical that narcissists not comprise the majority of the group.

Conclusion The results of three studies suggest that whether narcissism actually contributes to creative performance or whether the presence of that trait simply creates an unfounded impres- sion of creative talent may depend on the unit of analysis. Therefore, an important strength of this research is that we examined creativity at multiple levels to derive a more com- plete picture of how narcissism might contribute to (or mis- lead) creative problem-solving efforts. The results suggest that to capitalize on the narcissists in our midst, we should collaborate with them and encourage them to collaborate with each other. In so doing, groups could turn what is often con- sidered a decidedly negative trait into a valuable source of creative tension.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The authors received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article.

Notes

1. We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting this idea. 2. We included a control for sex in all of our analyses in Study 1 and

for the sex composition of each group in Study 3. The control for sex was not significant in any of the analyses, and all of our results held when the controls for sex were included. Therefore, we dropped that variable from our analyses in Studies 1 and 3.

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consen- sual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 997-1013.

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

1494 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(11)

Ames, D. R., Rose, P., & Anderson, C. P. (2006). The NPI-16 as a short measure of narcissism. Journal of Research in Personal- ity, 40, 440-450.

Anderson, C. A., & Kilduff, G. J. (2009). Why do dominant person- alities attain influence in face-to-face groups? The competence- signaling effects of trait dominance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 491-503.

Baer, M., Oldham, G. A., Jacobsohn, G. C., & Hollingshead, A. B. (2008). The personality composition of teams and creativity: The moderating role of team creative confidence. Journal of Creative Behavior, 42, 255-282.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari- able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., Neubert, M., & Mount, M. K. (1998). Relating member ability and personality to work team processes and team effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychol- ogy, 83, 377–391.

Baron, F. (1965). The psychology of creativity. In T. Newcombe (Ed.), New directions in psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 1-34). New York: Holt, Rhinehart & Winston.

Barron, F. (1967). Symbolic Equivalence Test. Unpublished test. Barron, F., & Harrington, D. (1981). Creativity, intelligence, and

personality. In M. Rosenzweig & L. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 439-476). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

Beersma, B., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2005). Conflict’s consequences: The effects of social motives on post-negotiation creative and convergent group functioning and performance. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 89, 345-357.

Bogart, L. M., Benotsch, B. G., & Pavlovic, J. L. (2004). Feeling superior but threatened: The relation of narcissism to social comparison. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26, 35-44.

Brunell, A. B., Gentry, W. A., Campbell, W. K., Hoffman, B. J., Kuhnert, K. W., & DeMarree, K. G. (2008). Leader emergence: The case of the narcissistic leader. Personality and Social Psy- chology Bulletin, 34, 1663-1676.

Carroll, L. (1987). A study of narcissism, affiliation and power motives among students in business administration. Psychologi- cal Reports, 61, 355-358.

Chatterjee, A., & Hambrick, D. C. (2007). It’s all about me: Nar- cissistic chief executive officers and their effects on company strategy and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 52, 351-386.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture and person: A sys- tems view of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.) The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 325- 339). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., & van Knippenberg, D. (2008). Motivated information processing in group judgment and decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 22-49.

Deutschman, A. (2005). Is your boss a psychopath? Fast Company, 96, 44-52.

Diehl, M., & Stroebe, W. (1987). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: Toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 497-509.

Dugosh, K. L., & Paulus, P. B. (2005). Cognitive and social com- parison processes in brainstorming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 313-320.

Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Mono- graphs, 58(Serial No. 270).

Elsbach, K. D., & Kramer, R. M. (2003). Assessing creativity in Hollywood pitch meetings: Evidence for a dual process model of creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 283-301.

Farwell, L., & Wohlwend-Lloyd, R. (2008). Narcissistic processes: Optimistic expectations, favorable self evaluations, and self- enhancing attributions. Journal of Personality, 66, 65-83.

Gersick, C. (1988). Time and transition in work teams: Toward a new model of group development. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 274-309.

Goncalo, J. A. & Kim, S. H. (2010). Distributive justice beliefs and group idea generation: Does a belief in equity facilitate produc- tivity? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 836-840.

Goncalo, J. A., & Staw, B. M. (2006). Individualism-collectivism and group creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci- sion Processes, 100, 96-109.

Guilford, J. P. (1956). The structure of intellect. Psychological Bul- letin, 33, 267-293.

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Helson, R., Roberts, B., & Agronick, G. (1995). Enduringness and change in creative personality and the prediction of occupa- tional creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1173-1183.

James, L. R. (1982). Aggregation bias in estimates of perceptual agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 219-229.

James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within group inter-rater reliability with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 85-98.

Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2001). The dynamic nature of conflict: A longitudinal study of intra-group conflict and group performance. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 238-251.

John, O. P., & Robbins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self perception: Individual differences in self enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 66, 206-219.

Kasof, J. (1995). Explaining creativity: The attributional perspec- tive. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 311-356.

Katz, A., & Giacommelli, L. (1982). The subjective nature of cre- ativity judgments. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 20, 17-20.

Kernis, M. H., & Sun, C. R. (1994). Narcissism and reactions to inter- personal feedback. Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 4-13.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Klein, K. J. (2000). A multi-level approach to theory and research in organizations: Contextual, temporal and emergent processes. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multi-level theory, research and methods in organizations:

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Goncalo et al. 1495

Foundations, extensions and new directions (pp. 3-90). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

MacDonald, R., & Wilson, G. (2005). Musical identities of profes- sional jazz musicians: A focus group investigation. Psychology of Music, 33, 395-417.

MacKinnon, D. W. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative tal- ent. American Psychologist, 17, 485-495.

Mannix, E., & Neale, M. (2005). What differences make a dif- ference? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 6, 31-55.

McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258-1265.

Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative pro- cess. Psychological Review, 26, 220-232.

Munkes, J., & Diehl, M. (2003). Matching or competition? Perfor- mance comparison processes in an idea generation task. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 305-320.

Nemeth, C. J., & Goncalo, J. A. (2005). Creative collaborations from afar: The benefits of independent authors. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 1-8.

Nemeth, C., & Wachtler, J. (1974). Creating the perceptions of consistency and confidence: A necessary condition for minority influence. Sociometry, 37, 529-540.

Nijstad, B. A., Stroebe, W., & Lodewijkx, H. F. M. (2002). Cog- nitive stimulation and interference in groups: Exposure effects in an idea generation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psy- chology, 38, 535-544.

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717-731.

Raskin, R. (1980). Narcissism and creativity: Are they related? Psy- chological Reports, 46, 55-60.

Raskin, R. N., & Shaw, R. (1988). Narcissism and the use of per- sonal pronouns. Journal of Personality, 56, 393-404.

Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal components analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 54, 890-902.

Rietzschel, E., Nijstad, B., & Stroebe, W. (2006). Productivity is not enough: A comparison of interactive and nominal brain-storming groups on idea generation and selection. Journal of Experimen- tal Social Psychology, 42, 244-251.

Rietzschel, E. F., Nijstad, B. A. & Stroebe, W. (2010). The selec- tion of creative ideas after individual idea generation: Choosing between creativity and impact. British Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 47-68.

Runco, M. A., & Bahleda, M. D. (1986). Implicit theories of artistic, scientific and everyday creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 20, 93-98.

Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal narcissists psychologically healthy? Self esteem matters. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 400-416.

Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-experimental studies: New procedures and recommenda- tions. Psychological Methods, 7, 422-445.

Simonton, D. K. (2003). Scientific creativity as constrained sto- chastic behavior: The integration of product, person and process perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 475-494.

Snyder, M. (1974). The self monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526-537.

Sutton, R. I., & Hargadon, A. (1996). Brainstorming groups in con- text: Effectiveness in a product design firm. Administrative Sci- ence Quarterly, 41, 685-718.

Taylor, C. W., & Barron, F. (1963). Scientific creativity. New York, NY: Wiley.

Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self efficacy: Its potential antecedents to creative performance. Academy of Man- agement Journal, 45, 1137-1146.

Wallace, H. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). The performance of narcissists rises and falls with perceived opportunity for glory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 819-834.

Ward, T. B. (1994). Structured imagination: The role of category struc- ture in exemplar generation. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 1-40.

Watson, P. J., Grisham, S. O., Trotter, M. V., & Biderman, M. D. (1984). Narcissism and empathy: Validity evidence for the nar- cissistic personality inventory. Journal of Personality Assess- ment, 48, 301-306.

Wiekens, C. J., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). I versus we: The effects of self-construal level on diversity. Social Cognition, 26, 368-377.

at CORNELL UNIV on November 15, 2010psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

View publication statsView publication stats

Please follow and like us: